Psychology 203

Calgary, Alberta, Canada
We are the Peer Mentors of Psychology 203: Psychology for Everyday Living at the University of Calgary. We will be sharing our thoughts about popular myths in psychology as they relate to each class.

Sunday 11 December 2011

Classes are Done!

Hello Psych 203 students:


It has been a pleasure acting as peer mentors for this class. We hope you enjoyed having our presence 'around' the classroom and if you have any questions prior to your final exam.... please do not hesitate to contact any of us. 


Have a great holiday season and good luck on your final exam.

Thursday 8 December 2011

Popular Myths in Psychology

Myths about Psychology and the Law

Myth: Virtually All People Who Confess to a Crime Are Guilty of It

by Amy Webber

Image: JonBenet Ramsey

It seems strange to think that anyone would confess to a crime they didn’t commit; however, the fact is that false confessions do occur, and it happens more often than you might think.

False confessions are especially likely to happen with high profile cases.  For example, over 200 people confessed to kidnapping world-famous aviator Charles Lindbergh’s son in 1932, and in the late 1940s, over 30 people confessed to the murder of Elizabeth Short, commonly known as the “Black Dahlia.”  In another famous case, John Mark Karr confessed to the 1996 murder of 6-year-old beauty queen JonBenet Ramsey, although his DNA didn’t match what was found at the crime scene.  These false confessions can cause serious problems in crime investigations, because they detract from finding the actual perpetrator.

But why would anyone confess to something they didn’t do?  In cases like these, it seems like the motivation might be fame.  Other reasons might include feeling a need for self-punishment to make up for some other wrong they’ve done in their lives, or trying to protect the real criminal, if it’s their child or spouse, for example.  Moreover, some people may be unable to differentiate fantasy from reality and thus might actually believe they did it. 

Further, in some cases, the coercive and deceptive tactics used by police officers during interrogations can lead to false confessions by innocent individuals.  During interrogations, police often use the “good cop/bad cop” technique, where they present false, made-up evidence against the suspect, making it sound like they have heaps of evidence pointing against the individual, when in reality, they do not.  The cop will then feign sympathy and provide reasons why the person might’ve done it; for example: “It’s completely understandable that you would act out in that situation, you had every right to be angry”, and offer them some sort of “deal” of a lesser punishment if they confess.  However, when someone is backed into a corner in this way, they might see no other way out than to confess to the crime, even if they didn’t do it.  This is referred to as a “compliant confession” where someone confesses in order to avoid a threat or gain a reward.  Other times, the person may actually start to believe they did it; for example, they may have been intoxicated the night of the crime, so when a police officer presents incriminating evidence against them (even if the evidence isn’t real, cops are allowed to make this stuff up for interrogation purposes), they may start to question their own innocence.  This is called an “internalized confession,” where they come to believe they actually did something they didn’t do.

What makes matters worse is that police officers tend to be highly confident in their ability to know whether someone is falsely confession, although research shows that police officers are actually more likely that other people to believe that a false confession is actually truthful.  This suggests they have a bias to believe that an innocent person is guilty.

Research shows that certain situational and personal characteristics can increase the likelihood of false confessions.  For example, people who are relatively young, suggestible, isolated from others, and who are confronted with strong evidence against them and questioned with intimidating and manipulative tactics, with prior criminal history and lack of legal counsel are more likely to confess to crimes they didn’t commit.

So what can be done to avoid false confessions?  Police should avoid revealing specific details about crime investigation cases to the media so that those who would be tempted to falsely confess in order to gain fame won’t be able to provide the essential details about the crime.  Also, some police departments videotape their interrogation interviews in order to ensure coercive tactics are not being used, and hopefully videotaping of interviews will be implemented more broadly.

References:
Lilienfeld, S. O., Lynn, S. J., Ruscio, J., & Beyerstein, B. L. (2010). 50 great myths of popular psychology: Shattering widespread misconceptions about human behavior. West Sussex, United Kingdom: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

Image from:
http://i.cdn.turner.com/trutv/trutv.com/graphics/photos/notorious_murders/famous/ramsey/Gallery/PG-Jonbenet-Ramsey%284%29.jpg

Tuesday 6 December 2011

Popular Myths in Psychology

Myths about Interpersonal Behavior

Myth: There is a ‘safety in numbers’: The more people present at an emergency, the greater the chances someone will intervene

by Alicia Kassian

It is scary to think that you could be murdered in front of bystanders and have them watch rather than intervene. Unfortunately, this is a more common occurrence than the human race would like to admit, and baffles most of the people who hear of it later. I bet you are thinking right now that you would not stand by and that you would help that person in need but in truth, probably not. In situations like murder, crime and injury, most of us are just not prepared to understand the situation and then change the outcome. The classic case is of Kitty Genovese that was murdered on a city street in New York and watched by some, heard by others but ultimately helped by….none. But why?!! Most psychologists like to think this is not a reflection of people’s natural behavior and is only because people are scared to act because of the unfamiliarity with the situation. I mean, when is the last time you saw a similar situation and knew what to do, what was going on, and how to change the outcome? Kitty is not the only one that has lost a life in the presence of others, In Manitoba an elderly women fell over into a snow bank on a busy street, she was not able to get up and froze to death. Let’s not forget about the Greyhound beheading incident not too long ago, rather graphic so if you want to know…google it. Probably the most famous bystander effect/obedience to authority would be Nazi Germany and psychologists believe that unfamiliarity with this type of situation and also seeing others also not intervening is part of the reason so many Jewish people were killed. This means that this could happen again! On a better note, psychology students that learn about the bystander effect are more likely to intervene than others so now you can be the ones that make the change.